CHECK OUT MY RECENTLY PUBLISHED PAPERS:
Exploring The Possibility Of An Elimination Algorithm as the Basis for Human Intuition: A Study of a Successful Expert System for Eye Disease Diagnosis
Unlocking The Science Of Emotions Through Pattern Recognition: Establishing Emotions As A Proper Field Of Study
Surgical destruction of the hippocampus caused Henry Gustav Molaison (HM) to lose his ability to remember events, which occurred even a few seconds earlier. But, HM retained memories for events long past.
While it is not the primary store of human memory, the hippocampus plays a crucial role in the consolidation of memories in extensive regions of the cortex. The hippocampus also supports a working memory, which enables the post-perceptual processing of information by the prefrontal regions.
The organ records memories of significant experiences of the mind in the sensory and recognition regions of the cortex, while providing strong space/time book marks for the recall of such memories. The organ recirculates the transient perceptions of the immediate past to the prefrontal regions for its current evaluation. This becomes a working memory, which protects the system from distraction and emotional reactivity and ensures quick and considered decisions and action plans.
During REM sleep, the hippocampus replays the space and time context of significant waking experiences. LTP circuits within the organ increase synaptic strength for such links. Neuronal reverberation, where linked nerve cells fire in rhythm, record the combinatorial patterns in all the linked groups of cells. Over many sleep/wake cycles, the organ spreads associative learning to extensive regions of the nervous system. With damage to the hippocampus, the nervous system loses its ability to bookmark, store and consolidate its episodic memories.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
What Is The Effect Of The Hippocampus?
The
hippocampus is a major component of the limbic system in the brains
of humans and other vertebrates. Like the cerebral cortex, with which
it is closely associated, it is a paired structure, with mirror-image
halves in the left and right sides of the brain. In humans and other
primates, the hippocampus is located inside the medial temporal lobe,
beneath the cortical surface. It resembles the folded back forelimbs
and webbed feet of the classical hippocampus - a sea monster with a
horse's forequarters and a fish's tail.
Damage to the
hippocampus can occur through age and disease. It is one of the first
regions to atrophy in Alzheimer's disease, causing memory problems
and disorientation. Damage can also result from oxygen starvation,
encephalitis, or medial temporal lobe epilepsy. In rats, stress
shortly after birth affects hippocampal function throughout life.
Humans who have experienced severe, long-lasting traumatic
stress, suffer atrophy of the hippocampus. In such cases, high levels
of cortisol in the blood stream damage the large population of
glucocorticoid receptors in the hippocampus. The effects show up in
post-traumatic stress disorder, and contribute to the hippocampal
atrophy in schizophrenia and severe depression. Damage to the
hippocampus does not affect the ability to learn to play a musical
instrument, or to solve certain types of puzzles, which involve
learning new motor or cognitive skills.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
Does Utilization Impact The Size Of The Hippocampus?
The
earliest role of the hippocampus was to remember the position of an
animal in space and to enable it to find the path to a hidden goal.
The organ helps to find shortcuts and new routes between familiar
places. As an example, London's taxi drivers are required by law to
know the most direct routes between numerous places in the city. A
study revealed that the relevant regions of the hippocampus were
larger in these drivers than in the general public. The organ also
had larger volumes in more experienced drivers.
In similar
species of animals, those with greater capacities for spatial memory
have larger hippocampal volumes. Bird species, which cache food, also
have a larger hippocampus. In humans, there is a reliable
relationship between the size of the hippocampus and memory
performance. Elderly people, suffering hippocampal shrinkage, tend to
perform less well on several memory tasks. Chores, which require
memory, tend to produce less hippocampal activation in the elderly.
While the hippocampus appears to be designed for spatial navigation,
the organ plays a significant role in enabling the storage of long
term memories.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
What Are The Types Of Human Memory?
Imagine that the physical and mental experiences of the mind
are stored by nerve cells as memories for contextual combinatorial
patterns. While most such memory is implicit, only a small proportion
of this vast store remains available for conscious recall. Most
people can indicate familiarity (implicit memory) with any one of
10,000 images displayed to them at 1 second intervals, without being
able to recall them (declarative memory). Amnesic patients show
implicit memory for experiences, while lacking a conscious knowledge
of them. They may guess which of two faces they saw most recently,
while claiming not to have seen them at all.
Representations
in working memory decay unless they are refreshed. Ben-Yakov and
Dudai link the hippocampus to sustained increases in neural activity,
creating a working memory, in contextual regions of the nervous
system, which are involved in execution of the task. If the mind is
engaged elsewhere, the task is less well remembered. The attention
load depends on the speed of the processing task. Adding digits every
half second places a higher load on the system than when adding them
every two seconds. The working memory does not require any inputs
other than attention.
Procedural memories enable a person to
play a musical instrument, or to ride a bike. Such memories, which
directly empower the motor system in real time, are acquired through
practice. Repetitive activity records the combinatorial memories in
nerve cells. Such memories cannot be consciously recalled, but are
available as a remembered ability. These memories assemble without
the assistance of the hippocampus.
Conscious recall becomes
possible, when attention is paid to a new and novel experience. The
hippocampus assists in the consolidation and storage of the
declarative memories of such experiences. Such memories may be both
for events and experiences as well as for semantic concepts (ideas
converted into words and sentences). The hippocampus uses its spacial
navigation competence to provide context for converting implicit
memories into episodic memories, which can be consciously recalled
after months and years. Damage to the hippocampus causes a loss of
the ability to acquire such memories.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
What Is The Basis For Neural Intelligence?
When
nerve cells fire to support a remembered mental function, the
presence of human memory is proved. A person who recalls a number
during calculation is recalling a working memory. The motor
systems in one who rides a bike recalls procedural memories. A
person, who recalls an event from the past uses his episodic
memory.
To understand human memory formation, imagine that complex intelligence depends on the memories of nerve
cells for dendritic firing combinations for signals from numerous
regions of the nervous system. Imagine that a neuron
fires, when it recognizes a combinatorial pattern in the array of its
receiving dendrites. The pattern may be a single signal in the array,
signals in a channel in the array, or a specific combinatorial
pattern of signals in the array. Nerve cells have memories for such combinatorial patterns.
Such memories were discovered
to be applied for hundreds of millions of years by the olfactory
sense (Nobel Prize 2004) for the instant identification of odors. Imagine that implicit memory for an odor is assembled,
when nerve cells routinely record the related firing combinations.
Such memories, which subsequently cause the cell to fire, may be
further consolidated through LTP, neural plasticity, or neuronal
reverberation.
Long term potentiation (LTP) enables neurons to
become sensitive to a single contextual signal. A neuron may also
grow new dendrites (neural plasticity), increasing accessibility to
active communication channels. Procedural memory is saved when linked
neurons fire repetitively during physical practice of the procedure.
Declarative memories for complex events are assembled through
neuronal reverberations, where groups of linked nerve cells fire in
rhythm. Later, contextual perceptual or emotional links recall those memories.
The Npas4 gene may play a role in the recording of
such combinatorial memories. Researchers at MIT discovered that by
knocking out the Npas4 gene in the DNA in the hippocampus,
neuroscientists created mice which kept entering cage compartments,
in spite of continued foot shocks. They had interfered with the
sequence of processes, which recorded memories of those painful
experiences. The motor memories, which enabled the animal to run,
remained.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
What Is The Role Of Place Cells?
The
nervous system constantly monitors its current location. The
hippocampus uses eye movement and head direction data as an inertial
compass to chart geographic movement and position. Visual and sound
information triangulate the location. These eye and ear coordinates
are mapped by head direction cells, grid cells, and border cells in
the entorhinal cortex and the closely linked hippocampus. These
regions were discovered to contain a neural map of the spatial
environment in rats. The firing cells in the arrays lack any spatial
topography in the representation. Cells lying next to each may have
uncorrelated spatial firing patterns.
While the location of
place fields may be random, it was discovered that finite array of
those cells chart the location of an animal in a chamber. These are
combinatorial patterns. Place cells are typically almost silent when
a rat is moving around outside the place field, but fire faster when
a rat reaches a place represented by the cell. The arrays of place
fields in young rats alter if the rat is moved into a different
environment, but reverts if the animal is returned to the same place.
But, the place fields frequently fail to "remap" in aged
rats.
The place fields record the place looked at by an animal
in four dimensions, including time. In humans, the cells even
indicate one's position in virtual reality spaces. The time
dimensioned combinatorial pattern of firing by the place cells in the
hippocampus trace the directions, objectives and movements of an
individual in his environment. Damage to the hippocapmus causes a
loss of this key reference point for episodic memories.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
What Is Neuronal Reverberation?
The
tactile, gustatory, olfactory, spatial, and motor activities produced
by the free exploration of novel objects trigger precise contextual
combinatorial links in multiple brain structures. When these active
groups of neurons fire in rhythm, among millions of silent ones, they
store combinatorial memories. Ann Graybiel recorded this process in
the basal ganglia of a monkey, while it learned to associate the
sound of a click with the availability of a sip of juice. Neuronal
reverberation, when connected groups of neurons fired rhythmically,
converted the action into a remembered drive for the animal. After
learning, the task did not require conscious effort for the
animal.
After a novel experience for an animal in a cage, the
correlation of neuronal reverberation between groups of cells
increases dramatically. This process repeats for several hours after
the learning experience. At the same, firing patterns related to
experiences with less new information (movements along the bare sides
of the cage) reduce. Cortical recognition identifies a new geographic
feature as being relevant to the goal of learning. Attention to the
feature intensifies the activity in the nerve cells, which perceive
and recognize. They fire in rhythm, triggering the acquisition of
combinatorial memories.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
What Is The Link Of The Hippocampus To Working Memory?
Joaquin
Fuster recorded the electrical activity of neurons in the PFC of
monkeys while they were doing a delayed matching task. In that task,
the monkey observed a bit of food being placed under one of two
identical looking cups. A shutter was then lowered for a variable
delay period, screening off the cups from the monkey's view. When the
shutter opened, the monkey was allowed to retrieve the food.
Successful retrieval required holding the location of the food in
working memory over the delay period. Fuster found neurons in the
PFC, the posterior parietal cortex, the thalamus, the caudate, and
the globus pallidus that fired mostly during the delay period,
suggesting that such firing represented the food location while it
was invisible.
Ben-Yakov and Dudai report a “stimulus
offset,” where events become “time locked,” after perception,
in the absence of sensory stimulation. In their (fMRI) experiments,
they identified bilateral hippocampus activity starting immediately
after stimulus presentation. Post-perceptual processing in the
absence of sensory stimulation was time-locked to the offset of
sensory input. The activation was found to increase for subsequently
remembered over forgotten content. Working memory maintenance may be
part of the long term memory maintenance process by the
hippocampus.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
How Do Sleep/Wake Cycles Affect Memory Formation?
Mammals
and birds enjoy long periods of dreamless slow-wave sleep (SW),
followed by short periods of rapid-eye-movement dreaming sleep (REM).
Sidarata Ribeiro suggests that the wake-sleep cycle promotes
propagation of memories outwards from the early coding sites.
Neuronal reverberation is strongest during SW sleep. The correlations
increase progressively during SW sleep, strengthening the memory
trace. SW sleep shortly after memory acquisition is critical for
memory consolidation. Sustained experience-dependent neuronal
reverberation can be detected in several cortical areas up to 48
hours after exposure to novel experiences.
Lack of such SW
sleep causes an irreversible loss of the recently acquired implicit
memory. Incoming sensory inputs during wake periods subdue the
neuronal reverberation linked to past novel experience. Memory
formation occurs during sleep from the absence of sensory
interference. The neocortical reverberation decays rapidly within one
hour of memory trace formation. Brain activity after acquisition of
new data has been shown to be proportional to memory acquisition in
rats and humans and to quantitatively predict learning.
The
Hippocampus And Severe Memory Loss
What Is REM Sleep?
A
significant consolidation of memory takes place during REM sleep.
Nature has provided a mechanism to replay the space/time context
through rapid eye movements during sleep, causing persistent neuronal
reverberation in the cortex, hippocampus, putamen, and thalamus.
Incremental learning continues several nights after memory
acquisition due to the progressive recruitment of larger neuronal
networks over time. The hippocampus also has mechanisms, which
progressively disengage the organ from its older memory consolidation
processes.
Neuronal reverberation takes place in both SW and
REM sleep. But, the physical movements of the eye distinguish the REM
sleep process, critical to the learning experience. The added neural
firing by the eye muscles may contribute to access (or store) data by
identifying its space/time context. Subconscious eye movements often
accompany search processes of the mind - say, the preparation of a
shopping list. The eye movements may enable REM sleep to achieve
greater knowledge consolidation in less time than SW sleep.